Does Eye Black Actually Do Anything?
For those that don’t know “eye black” is the substance used to make the dark markings athletes place under their eyes that are supposed to prevent glare from outside light sources, such as stadium lights and the Sun. It’s also supposed to help with contrast recognition (being able to tell the color of objects that make them distinguishable from objects in the background)- like when a baseball player is trying to see a ball coming at 100 mph when there is a gray background, such as a pitcher’s uniform. Before we talk about exactly how eye black is thought to work, it’s important to understand what light is and the effect it has on our eyes.
Light is merely electromagnetic radiation. Electromagnetism is one of the four fundamental forces in the universe, the others being gravity (for now) and the nuclear weak and strong forces. This “light” radiation comes in many forms you probably recognize- from the long wavelength (low frequency radio waves, microwaves, and infrared waves), to the shorter wavelength (higher frequency ultraviolet waves, x-ray waves, and Gamma-ray waves). Right in the middle of these, is a sliver of the spectrum that we can see with our human eyes, namely visible light waves. The wavelengths we are able to see range from about 380 nanometers (your purples and blues) to about 740 nanometers (your reds).
Inside our eyes we have two types of cells that detect light (photoreceptors). They are known as rods and cones. There are many more rods in our eyes than cones (about 120 million rods compared to 6-7 million cones). The rods are more sensitive, but the cones allow us to see color. There are three types of color reception in our cones (thus, 3 primary colors).
How light is absorbed or reflected has to do with how the specific frequencies of light affects the electrons that are attached to the atoms within the molecules of the cells. Electrons travel around atoms at specific frequencies. When those frequencies match the frequencies of light waves hitting the object, that object will absorb the frequency of light. When it doesn’t match, that object will reflect those frequencies back. The rejected frequencies are what our eyes perceive as objects and color. Thus, if you see something that is green, that object is absorbing all the frequencies of light, except the ones associated with green. When an object appears white, it is reflecting nearly all light frequencies back at your eyes, and not absorbing any. When an object appears black, it is absorbing almost all light frequencies and reflecting none back.
When the objects we see, like a baseball, are perceived by our eyes, and there is another light source that is greater than the one illuminating the object (baseball), it produces a glare that is also picked up by our eyes. This glare also affects how the electron and light frequencies work to create an image. This scattering of light makes the object we are looking at appear less defined, playing havoc on anyone who makes a living at using their vision. If you’ve ever driven at night and had trouble seeing when there are oncoming headlights, you know what I’m talking about.
The glare associated with trying to perceive objects around you, when there are intense light sources like the Sun, or stadium lights, is known as “disability glare”. According the International Commission on Illumination (CIE), disability glare is defined as “A visual condition in which there is excessive contrast or an inappropriate distribution of light sources that disturbs the observer or limits the ability to distinguish details and objects”.
Basically, the rods and cones in our eyes begin to perceive light sources from different angles. These differing light sources begin to cause the rods and cones to be unable to distinguish specific shapes and objects, much like trying to hear someone talk over a loud radio.
The idea behind eye black is to reduce the amount of glare that reaches your eyes. Because black colors absorb most light frequencies, it is thought that the excessive light coming from sources we are not looking at will be absorbed by the black color. Placing the black just below our eyes will, in effect, reduce the glare, thereby increasing the definition and contrast of the objects we are looking at.
The theory is sound, but does it work? The short answer is, probably. There has been only one seemingly legitimate study that I could find that has tested this theory, done by Brian M. De Broff, MD and Patricia J. Pahk, MD in 2003; it was published in the Journal of American Medical Association. They tested the difference between eye black, commercially available black stickers, and petroleum jelly. What they found was that you did get an increase in your ability to perceive contrast differences in objects with both the eye black and the commercially available stickers. They also found that the eye black was better than the stickers. The ancillary evidence of the popularity of eye black, also speaks to its ability to work. Being someone who likes irrefutable scientific evidence before he makes any statement as to the accuracy of a theory, I must point out this study is the only one done, and it had a very small sample group (only 46 people).
So not quite as definitive as I normally like to be, but seems reasonable enough given that the theory behind eye black is a good one and the only scientifically done study on the subject proved positive. (For those who put stock in somewhat non-scientific studies, MythBusters also agreed with the study after doing their own experiments.) The small sample size and the lack of any major studies that have replicated the results makes me hesitant to say that it absolutely works, but I’ll update this article if that changes in future. If nothing else, eye black sure makes a person look scary and intimidating, which will also help on the field of battle!
If you liked this article and the Bonus Facts below, you might also like:
- Why We Divide Time Up Into Seconds, Minutes, and Hours
- The Origin of the Word “Soccer”
- Does Sex Before an Athletic Event Really Hinder Performance?
- What Causes Red Eyes in Photographs
- Reading in Dim Light Will Not Damage Your Vision
Bonus Facts:
- Reducing the glare from the sun is nothing new. The first known anti-glare devices were used by Eskimos about 2000 years ago. They made goggles from ivory and wood that had horizontal slots in them. This allowed them peripheral vision while blocking light that was reflected by snow and ice.
- The first known commercially available means of blocking glare came from the Sears, Roebuck and Company. They began selling sunglasses in 1886.
- Eye black grease is made from mixing carbon, paraffin, and beeswax.
- According to the advertisements on commercially available anti-glare stickers, the appropriate way to place the stickers is ½ inch below the eyelid, on the cheekbone.
- The first person credited with using eye black was Andy Farkas of the Washington Redskins. He was photographed with it on in a game against the Philadelphia Eagles in1942. It was thought that players at that time burned cork and smeared the ashes on their cheeks.
Share the Knowledge! |
I enjoyed the article. I disagree on some points however.
I was with you until I reached paragraph 5, which starts with “when the objects we see…”. I’m not sure I follow the line of thinking here.
If I read the article correctly, light (lets say, a given photon) on a collision course with the retina can be redirected toward the “eyeblack”? What physical phenominon allows eyeblack to attract light toward it?
I agree that eyeblack (because it is black) absorbs visible light, but this only applies to light that was headed towards the area covered by the eyeblack in the first place (light that would never have reached the retina, and as a result, never would have been seen anyway).
I would agree also that eyeblack can absorb light and therefore prevent that light from being reflected off of the cheeks, but because the cheeks can’t be seen by the eyes (I can’t see my cheeks, no matter how far down I try to look), reflection doesn’t seem to be a factor.
I take that back. I CAN see my nose. It might make some sense I guess, if athletes were to put black paint on their noses, but that probably won’t ever happen because it would look silly.
Disclaimer : I feel eyeblack is purely cosmetic.
Our noses are always in peripheral view, we just subconsciously block it out
I’ve got chubby cheeks and if I look, I can see my cheeks. Mostly I subconsciously block it (like we all do with our noses) but I can see my cheeks in my peripheral vision. Though it’s far more pronounced the wider I’m smiling.
Under eye blackout is really silly unless the athlete wearing the make up believes it will somehow intimidate opponent athletes. Kids in high school are likely to wear this blackout because they think it looks cool and makes them manly. When I played high school lacrosse our coach decided one day that we should wear under eye black make up. I noticed zero improvement in vision. My girlfriend laughed when I got off the field. It was obviously bs. Professional athletes that wear eye makeup?
“I agree that eyeblack (because it is black) absorbs visible light,”
You answered your own question. Also youtube the mythbusters take on this in the link the author provided.
Disclaimer : I feel eyeblack is purely cosmetic.
Thank god sciences doesnt care about your “feelings”, because right now I “feel” like you might be retarded.
At the end of the day they provided cases studies and you provided feelings.
Sigh. I really shouldn’t bother, but here goes. The fact that black absorbs light is only part of the problem. You neglect to consider that the black region must be in line-of-sight with the eye, which is not the case with eye black. One might as well say black hair (which also absorbs light) cuts down on glare too. After all, it’s also near the eyes, and apparently proximity is a factor. Your argument is that mythbusters told you so? I’m not sure I’ve ever heard a scientist say that before. I am challanging this convention with a simple thought experiment. If you wander back to this page, consider reading it again and specifically addressing the sight-of-sight aspect of my argument. Cheers!
My understanding is that eye black absorbs light that would otherwise reflect off the player’s cheeks. I’m sceptical about the efficacy of this (which is why I am here), but it at least addresses your question about line-of-sight.
Having played football under bright lights, I can attest to the fact that sweaty cheeks indeed reflect light up into your eyes like a glow that is really annoying and makes it hard to see. You either have to keep wiping the sweat off (not easy with a helmet on and wearing gloves), or wear eye black, which does indeed solve this problem.
Actually Babe Ruth is credited in using eye black in the 30s. Andy Farkas popularized it. While I think it may have some potential use in preventing light reflecting off cheeks and sweat I cannot agree that it really has any value as a viable option. It still is up for debate as to its effectiveness and has had several experiments done since the one you mentioned. However, the one you referenced was not done as a blind study so bias of those within the study played a role thus making that experiment invalid at all. I appreciate the effort though on your minimal research, Wikipedia is a far more effective source than given credit.
In my younger years I too played under bright lights as well as glaring sun. The eye black absolutely makes a difference. Perhaps there are those who cannot see any part of their cheeks, but I can see a small bit of my cheeks at the bottom edge of my sight. This does reflect light into my eyes and glare is definitely reduced by using eye black (can’t speak to the stickers – I’ve never tried those). Regardless of anyone’s feelings on the subject, I’ll rely on my direct personal experience the confirms the research.
I can’t see my cheeks under my eyelids more than just a sliver when squinting. However, the sides of my nose are clearly in view all the time. The fact that nobody puts eye black on the sides of their noses proves that the “reflection off the cheek” theory is invalid. It’s about looking cool, which is why nobody paints the sides of their nose. Haha.
This. Finally someone with some sense to the obvious.
You only see North American athletes wearing eye black, so I always thought it was more fashion than function.
As a person who wore eye black in my younger days playing baseball and softball I believe it worked, now I’m no scientists but if something you do makes you feel better that converts into a more positive mindset or confidence and in competition confidence is power. Like a new shirt it won’t change your physique but you might feel better and walk with a bit more bounce in your step, so if you think it works then it does work.
If you can’t see the eye black, then you couldn’t have seen the light it was supposedly absorbing. Duh.
Those in the study knew what they were wearing and so makes this meaningless as knowing can influence your answer
Inte4resting that players do not put eyeblack on the part of their noses that is in their field of vision. Also that their face masks are not routinely painted flat black.
FWIW, on racing sailboats, some crews paint the inner surface of the bow railing flat black, so it does not reflect the green and red sidelights when sailing at night, which makes it easier to see the waves in front of the boat.